Conclusion
This work suggests an account of both how and why children form the argument structure generalizations they do. The paper’s main theoretical contribution is to explicitly relate generally recognized facts about language acquistion to generally recognized facts about categorization. Many researchers in language acquisition allude to general categorization processes, but very little in the way of specifics have been offered. We argue that children initially generalize at the level of specific verbs plus argument slots (Tomasello’s “verb islands”) because the verb in an argument frame is an excellent predictor of overall sentence meaning. We argue further that children eventually generalize away from specific verbs to form more abstract argument structure constructions because the argument frame or construction has roughly equivalent cue validity as a predictor of overall sentence meaning than the morphological form of the verb, and has much greater category validity. That is, initially children attend to the complex of cues: that of verb and construction; eventually, attention focuses on the construction because the construction has equivalent cue validity and much greater category validity as compared with individual verbs, given the task of sentence comprehension. The construction is at least as reliable and much more available.
We further argue that the input is structured in such a way as to make the generalization from verb islands to argument structure constructions straightforward. The paper’s main empirical contribution is the finding that one particular verb accounts for the lion’s share of instances of each argument frame considered in an extensive corpus study on the Bates et al. (1988) corpus, in both mothers’ and 28 month old children’s speech. The dominance of a single verb in the construction facilitates the association of the meaning of the verb in the construction with the construction itself, allowing learners to get a “fix” on the construction’s meaning. In this way, grammatical constructions may arise developmentally as generalizations over lexical items in particular patterns.
Works Cited
Ackerman, F., & Webelhuth, G. (1988). A theory of predicates: CSLI Publications.
Akhtar, N. (1999). Acquiring word order: Evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structure. Journal of Child Language, 26(2), 339-356.
Akhtar, N., & Tomasello, M. (1997). Young children's productivity with word order and verb morphology. Developmental Psychology, 33(6), 952-965.
Anderson, J. R. (1991). The adaptive nature of human categorization. Psychological Review, 98(3), 409-429.
Anh, W.-K., & Medin, D. L. (1992). A two-stage model of category construction. Cognitive Science, 16(1), 81-121.
Aronoff. (1976). Word Formation in Generative Grammar. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Avrahami, J., Kareev, Y., Bogot, Y., Caspi, R., Dunaevsky, S., & Lerner, S. (1997). Teaching by examples: Implications for the process of category acquisition. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 50A(3), 586-606.
Baker, C. L. (1979). Syntactic theory and the projection problem. Linguistic Inquiry, 10(4), 533-581.
Bates, E., Bretherton, I., & Snyder, L. (1988). From First Words to Grammar: Individual Differences and Dissociable Mechanisms. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bates, E., & Goodman, J. (1998). Grammar form the lexicon. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), The Emergence of Language (pp. 197-212). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bates, E., & MacWhinney, B. (1987). Competition, variation, and language learning. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of Language Acquisition (pp. 157-193). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Bencini, G., & Goldberg, A. (2000). The contribution of argument structure constructions to sentence meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43(4), 640-651.
Bowerman, M. (1973). Early Syntactic Development: A Cross-linguistic Study with Special Reference to Finnish. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Bowerman, M. (1982). Reorganizational processes in lexical and syntactic development. In E. Wanner & L. R. Gleitman (Eds.), Language Acquisition: The State of the Art (pp. 319-346). New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bowerman, M. (1990). Mapping Thematic Roles onto Syntactic Functions: Are Children Helped by Innate Linking Rules? Linguistics, 28(6 (310), 1253-1289.
Braine, M. D. S. (1976). Children's First Word Combinations (Vol. 4 (1)).
Bresnan, J. (1982). The Mental Representation of Grammatical Relations. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Bresnan, J., & Kanerva, J. (1989). Locative inversion in Chichewa: A case study in the factorization of grammar. Linguistic Inquiry, 20(????), 1-50.
Brooks, P., & Tomasello, M. (1999). How children constrain their argument structure constructions. Language, 75(4), 720-738.
Brown, R. (1973). A First Language: The Early Stages. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.
Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. (1956). A Study of Thinking. New York: Wiley.
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular Morphology and the Lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10(5), 425-455.
Bybee, J., Perkins, R., & Pagliuca, W. (1992). The Evolution of Grammar: Tense, Aspect, and Modality in the Languages of the World. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Bybee, J. L. (1985). Morphology: A Study of the Relation between Meaning and Form: John Benjamins Publishing Company.
Carey, S., & Bartlett, E. (1978). Acquiring a single new word.Unpublished manuscript, Stanford: CA.
Cartwright, T. A., & Brent, M. R. (1997). Syntactic categorization in early language acquisition: Formalizing the role of distributional analysis. Cognition, 63(2), 121-170.
Choi, S. (1999). Early Development of Verb Structures and caregiver Input in Korean: Two case studies. 3(2-3), 241-265.
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects and the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Chomsky, N. (1980). The linguistic approach. In M. Piatelli-Palmarini (Ed.), Language and Learning (pp. 109-130). Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press.
Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and Problems of Knowledge: The Managua Lectures. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Clark, E. V. (1978). Discovering what words can do. Paper presented at the Papers from the parasession on the lexicon.
Clark, E. V. (1996). Early verbs, event types and inflections (Vol. 9). Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Clark, E. V., & Clark, H. H. (1979). When nouns surface as verbs. Language, 55(4), 767-811.
Culicover, P. W. (1999). Syntactic Nuts: hard Cases, Syntactic Theory and Language Acquisition reviewed by John R Taylor. Cognitive Linguistics, 10(3), 251-261.
Davis, A. R. (1996). Lexical Semantics and Linking in the Hierarchical Lexicon. Unpublished Ph.D Dissertation, Stanford University, Stanford: CA.
De Villiers, J. G. (1985). Learning how to use verbs: Lexical coding and the influence of the input. Journal of Child Language, 12(3), 587-595.
Deacon, T. W. (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-Evolution of Language and the Brain.
Dennis, S., & Kruschke, J. K. (1998). Shifting attention in cued recall. Australian Journal of Psychology, 50(3), 131-138.
Dowty, D. (1991). Thematic proto-roles and argument selection. Language, 67(3), 547-619.
Elio, R., & Anderson, J. R. (1984). The effects of information order and learning mode on schema abstraction. Memory and Cognition, 12(1), 20-30.
Elman, J., Bates, E., Johnson, M., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Parisi, D., & Plunkett, K. (1996). Rethinking Innateness: A Connectionist Perspective on Development. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Estes, W. K. (1986). Array models for category learning. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 500-548.
Fillmore, C. J., Kay, P., Kathol, A., & Michaelis, L. (Unpublished Manuscript). Construction Grammar.Unpublished manuscript, University of California, Berkeley.
Fisher, C. (1996). Structural limits on verb mapping: The role of analogy in children's interpretations of sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 31(1), 41-81.
Fisher, C. (2000). From form to meaning: A role for structural alignment in the acquisition of language. Adv Child Developmental Behavior, 27, 1-53.
Fisher, C., Gleitman, H., & Gleitman, L. R. (1991). On the semantic content of subcategorization frames. Cognitive Psychology, 23(3), 331-392.
Ford, M. B. J. K. R. M. (1982). A Competence-Based Theory of Syntactic Closure. MIT Press. Cambridge.
Garnsey, S. M., Pearlmutter, N. J., Myers, E., & Lotocky, M. (1997). The contributions of verb bias and plausibility to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 37(1), 58-93.
Gentner, D. (1982). Why nouns are learned before verbs: Linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. In S. A. Kuzcaj II (Ed.), Language Development, Vol 2: Language, Thought, and Culture (Vol. 2, pp. 301-334). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Gentner, D., & Medina, J. (1998). Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition, 65(2-3), 263-297.
Gillette, J., Gleitman, H., Gleitman, L. R., & Lederer, A. (1998). Human simulations of vocabulary learning: University of Pennsylvania.
Givón, T. (1971). Historical syntax and synchronic morphology: An archaeologist's field trip. Chicago Linguistic Society, 7, 384-415.
Gleitman, L. (1994). The Structural Sources of Verb Meanings. In P. Bloom (Ed.), Language Acquisition: core readings: MIT Press.
Goldberg, A. E. (1992). The inherent semantics of argument structure: The case of the English ditransitive construction. Cognitive Linguistics, 3(1), 37-74.
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: A Construction Grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: Chicago University Press.
Goldberg, A. E. (1996). Optimizing constraints and the Persian complex predicate. Berkely Linguistic Society, ????
Goldberg, A. E. (1998). The emergence of the semantics of argument structure constructions. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), The Emergence of Language (pp. 197-212). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Grégoire, A. (1937). L'apprentissage du langage (Vol. I). Paris: Droz.
Grice, H. P. (1975). Logic and Conversation. In P. Cole & J. Morgan (Eds.), Syntax and Semantics 3: Speech Acts (pp. 41-58). New York: Academic Press.
Grimshaw, J. (1981). Form, function and the language acquisition device. In C. L. Baker & J. J. McCarthy (Eds.), The Logical Problem of Language Acquisition. Cambridge: MIT Press.
Grimshaw, J. (1990). Argument Structure. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Gropen, J., Epstein, T., & Schumacher, L. (1997). Context-sensitive verb learning: Children's ability to associate contextual information with the argument of a verb. Cognitive Linguistics, 8(2), 137-182.
Gropen, J., Pinker, S., Hollander, M., Goldberg, R., & Wilson, R. (1989). The learnability and acquisition of the dative alternation in English. Language, 65(2), 203-257.
Gross, M. (1975). Language.
Healy, A. F., & Miller, G. A. (1970). The verb as the main determinant of sentence meaning. Psychonomic Science, 20, 372.
Heine, B. (1993). Auxiliaries: Cognitive forces and grammaticalization. New York: Oxford University Press.
Hintzman, D. L. (1986). "Schema abstraction" in a multiple-trace memory model. Psychological Review, Vol 93(4), Oct 1986, 1411-1428 American Psychological Assn , US.
Hirsh-Pasek, K., Golinkoff, R. M., & Naigles, L. (1996). Young Children's Use of Syntactic Frames to Derive Meaning. In K. Hirsh-Pasek & R. M. Golinkoff (Eds.), The Origins of Grammar: Evidence from Early Language Comprehension (Vol. MIT Press). Cambridge, MA.
Holland, J. H., Holyoak, K. J., & Thagard, R. E. R. (1989). Induction: Processes of Inference, Learning and Discovery. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Homa, D., Dunbar, S., & Nohre, L. (1991). Instance frequency, categorization, and the modulating effect of experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, Vol 17(3), May 1991, 1444-1458 American Psychological Assn, US, http://www apa org.
Hooper, P. J., & Thompson, S. A. (1980). Transitivity in Grammar and Discourse. Language, 56(2), 251-299.
Horn, L. R. (1984). Towards a new taxonomy for pragmatic inference: Q-based and R-based implicature. In D. Schiffrin (Ed.), GURT '84: Meaning form and use in context. Washington: Georgetown University Press.
Hovav, & Levin. (1998). Building Verb Meanings. In M. Butt & W. Geuder (Eds.), The projection of Arguments: Lexical and Compositional Factors. Stanford: CA: CSLI Publications.
Ingram, D., & Thompson, W. (1996). Early syntactic acquisition in German: Evidence for the Modal Hypothesis. Language, 72(1), 97-120.
Jackendoff, R. (1983). Semantics and Cognition. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Jackendoff, R. (to appear).
Janda, R. D. (1990). Frequency, markedness and morphological change: On predicting the spread of noun-plural -s in Modern High German and West Germanic. ESCOL, 7, 136-153.
Kay, P., & Fillmore, C. J. (1999). Grammatical constructions and linguistic generalizations: The What's X doing Y? construction. Language, 75(1), 1-34.
Kersten, A. W., & Billman, D. (1997). Event category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23(3), 638-658.
Kruschke, J. K. (1996). Base rates in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 22(1), 3-26.
Kuno, S. (1972). Functional sentence perspective: A case study from Japanese and English. Linguistic Inquiry, 3(3), 269-320.
Lambrecht, K. (1994). Information Structure and Sentence Form: Cambridge University Press.
Landau, B., & Gleitman, L. R. (1985). Language and Experience: Evidence from a Blind Child. Cambridge: Mass: Harvard University Press.
Leake, D. B., & Ram, A. (1995). Learning, goals, and learning goals: A perspective on goal-driven learning. Artificial Intelligence Review, 9(6), 387-422.
Levin, B. (1993). English verb Classes and Alternations. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Lieven, E. V. M., Pine, J. M., & Baldwin, G. (1997). Lexically-based learning and early grammatical development. Journal of Child Language, 24(1), 187-219.
MacDonald, M. C., Pearlmutter, N. J., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1993). The Lexical Nature of Syntactic Ambiguity Resolution.Unpublished manuscript, USC.
MacWhinney, B. (1978). The Acquisition of Morphophonology (Vol. 43). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
MacWhinney, B. (1982). Basic syntactic processes. In S. A. K. II (Ed.), Language Development (Vol. Syntax and Semantics Vol 1). New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
MacWhinney, B. (1995). The CHILDES Project: Tools for analyzing talk (Second ed.). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Maddox, W. T. (1995). Base-Rate Effects in Multidimensional Perceptual Categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21(2), 288-301.
Markman, A. B., & Gentner, D. (1993). Structural alignment during similarity comparisons. Cognitive Psychology, 25(4), 431-467.
Matsumoto, Y. (1992). Abstract Motion and English and Japanese Verbs.Unpublished manuscript, Tokyo Christian University.
Medin, D. L., & Edelson, S. M. (1988). Problem structure and the use of base-rate information from experience. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 117(1), 68-85.
Medin, D. L., Wattenmaker, W. D., & Hampson, S. E. (1987). Family resemblance conceptual cohesiveness, and category construction. Cognitive Psychology, 12(2), 242-279.
Morris, W. C., & Cottrell, G., W. (1999). The empirical acquisition of grammatical relations. Paper presented at the Proceedings of the Cognitive Science Society Meeting.
Murphy, G. L. (1982). Cue validity and levels of categoryization. Psychological Bulletin, 91(1), 174-177.
Naigles, L. (1990). Children use syntax to learn verb meanings. Journal of Child Language, 357-374.
Naigles, L. R., & Hoff-Ginsberg, E. (1998). Why are some verbs learned before other verbs? Effects of input frequency and structure on children's early verb use. JCL, 25, 95-120.
Newmeyer, F. (1998). Language Form and Language Function. Cambridge: Mass: MIT Press.
Ninio, A. (1996, July 1996). Pathbreaking verbs in semantic development. Paper presented at the presented at the 7th Internatioanl Congress for Child Language, Istanbul, Turkey.
Ninio, A. (1999). Pathbreaking verbs in syntactic development and the question of prototypical transitivity. JCL, 26(619-653).
Nosofsky, R. (1988). Similarity, frequency and category representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 14(1), 54-65.
Olguin, R., & Tomasello, M. (1993). Twenty-five-month-old children do not have a grammatical category of verb. Cognitive Development, 8(3), 245-272.
Park, T.-Z. (1977). Emerging language in Korean children.Unpublished manuscript, Bern: Institute of Psychology.
Pinker, S. (1981). Comments on the paper by Wexler. In C. L. Baker & J. J. McCarthy (Eds.), The Logical Problem of Language Acquisition. Cambridge: Mass: Harvard University Press.
Pinker, S. (1984). Language Learnability and Language Development: Harvard University Press.
Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and Cognition: The Acquisition of Argument Structure. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press/Bradford Books.
Pinker, S. (1994). The Language Instinct. New York: William Morrow and Co.
Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1991). U-Shaped learning and frequency-effects in a multilayerd perceptron: Implications for child language acquisition. Cognition, 38(1), 43-102.
Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1993). From rote learning to system building: Acquiring verb morphology in children and connectionist nets. Cognition, 48(1), 21-69.
Pollard, C., & Sag, I. (1987). Information-based syntax and semantics. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications.
Posner, M. I., Goldsmith, R., & Welton, K. E. J. (1967). Perceived distance and the classification of distorted patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 73(1), 28-38.
Posner, M. I., & Keele, S. W. (1968). On the genesis of abstract ideas. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 77(3.1), 353-363.
Reddy, M. (1979). The conduit metaphor. In A. Ortony (Ed.), Metaphor and thought (pp. 284-324). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Regehr, G., & Brooks, L. R. (1995). Category organization in free classification: The organizing effect of an array of stimuli. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 21(2), 347-363.
Reinhart, T. (1982). Pragmatics and linguistics: An analysis of sentence topics. Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University Linguistics Club.
Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family Resemblances: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7(573-605).
Ross, B. H., & Makin, V. S. (1999). Prototype versus exemplar models, The Concept of Cognition. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press.
Sampson, G. (1997). Educating Eve: The 'Language Instinct' Debate. New York: Cassell.
Sanchés, M. (1978). On the Emergence of Multi-Element Utterances in the Child's Japanese.Unpublished manuscript, Austin, Texas.
Schlesinger, I. M. (1982). Steps to Language: Toward a Theory of Language Acquisition. Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Seger, C. A. (1994). Implicit learning. Psychological Bulletin, 115(2), 163-196.
Sethuraman, N., Goldberg, A. E., & Goodman, J. (1997). Using the semantics associated with syntactic frames for interpretation without the aid of non-linguistic context. Paper presented at the Twenty-seventh Annual Child Language Research Forum.
Silverstein, M. (1976). Hierarchy of features and ergativity. In R. Dixon (Ed.), Grammatical categories in Australian languages. New York: Humanities Press.
Slobin, D. I. (1985). Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity. In D. I. Slobin (Ed.), A Crosslinguistic Study of Language Acquisition (Vol. 2: Theoretical Issues). Hillsdale, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Smith, L. B. (1981). Importance of overall similarity of objects for adults' and children's classifications. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7(4), 811-824.
Smith, L. B., Jones, S. S., & Landau, B. (1996). Naming in young children: A dumb attentional mechanism? Cognition, 60(2), 143-171.
Song, J. J. (1996). Causatives and Causation. New York: Addison, Wesley, Longman.
Talmy, L. (Ed.). (1976). Semantic Causative Types (Vol. 6). New York: Academic Press.
Tomasello, M. (1992). First verbs: A Case Study of Early Grammatical Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Tomasello, M. (2000). Do young children have adult syntactic competence? Cognition, 74(3), 209-253.
Tomasello, M., Aktar, N., Dodson, K., & Rekau, L. (1997). Differential productivity in young children's use of nouns and verbs. Journal of Child Language, 24(2), 373-387.
Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Kello, C. (1993). Verb specific constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical preference from garden-paths. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 19(3), 528-553.
Windfuhr, G. (1979). Persian Grammar. New York: Mouton.
Wisniewski, E. J. (1995). Prior knowledge and functionally relevant features in concept learning. Memory and Cognition, 21(2), 449-468.
Wolfenden, S. N. (1929). Outlines of Tibeto-Burman linguistic morphology. London: Royal Asiatic Society.
Zipf, G. K. (1935). The Psycho-Biology of Language. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Dostları ilə paylaş: |