STATEMENT BY AUTHOR This dissertation has been submitted in partial fulfillment of requirements for an advanced degree at The University of Arizona and is deposited in the University Library to be made available to borrowers under rules of the library. Brief quotations from this dissertation are allowable without special permission, provided that accurate acknowledgment of source is made. Requests for permission for extended quotation from or reproduction of this manuscript in whole or in part may be granted by the head of the major department or the Dean of the Graduate College when in his or her interests the proposed use of the material is in the interests of scholarship. In all other instances, however, permission must be obtained from the author. SIGNED: Acknowledgments
I wish to thank the people who have been the most help to me in the ends and means of this dissertation. Of utmost importance has been Mike Hammond. Without his knowledge, encouragement, persistence friendship, and bridge playing, this document surely would have never been finished. Many of the ideas in here were inspired by him. I am also grateful for the help from my other committee members, Terry Langendoen and Dick Demers. They were always ready to discuss my ideas and share my enthusiasm for the variable-position phenomenon. I also thank all the Linguistics graduate students I have spent time with during my tenure here. Every one of them has added something to my view of the Linguistic world. I would especially like to thank Diane Ohala, Amy Fountain, Pat Perez and Wendy Wiswall for their knowledge, ideas, proofreading skills and friendship. They have been much more than my colleagues. I would also like to thank my husband, Bruce Walsh for his laughter, loving, nagging and housekeeping skills. He has been both a help and a delight during this process. My undying gratitude goes out to Nan Crandall, Matt Shimel, Jennifer Wateuma, Fred Matter and Maria Teresa Velez for covering for me at work so that I take time off to finish my dissertation. Finally, I would like to thank Trouble, Abby, Mouth, Rusty, Scamper, PeeWee, Duffy and Spot for keeping me sane during this process.
This dissertation is dedicated to my parents,
Gordon and Marjorie Fulmer,
who taught me with their love and understanding,
everything I will ever need to know.
Thank you.
1. Introduction 8
Background 16
Afar 17
Phonemics 18
Sounds 18
Co-occurrence Restrictions 25
Vowel Quality 26
Consonants 31
Morphology 38
General Morphology 39
Verb Classes 47
Theoretical Background 56
Introduction to Optimality Theory 57
Correspondence Theory 72
Generalized Alignment 76
Morphological Planes 78
The Morphemic Tier Hypothesis 79
Planes in Concatenative Languages 96
Morphological Planes and Optimality Theory 102
Huave 107
Dissertation Overview 112
2. RELEVANT PHONOLOGY 116
Syllables 116
The Structure of Syllables 117
Closed-Syllable Long Vowel Reduction 134
Word Final Long Vowel Reduction 141
Superheavy Syllables 147
Long Vowels in Word-Final Position 164
The Minimal Word 164
168
Prosodic Vowels 169
The Deletion of /y/ 178
The Absence of [y] 186
Phonological Distribution 186
Morphological Alternations 189
Insertion or Deletion 192
Summary 199
3. A MULTIPLANAR MODEL 203
The Data: The Person and Plural Markers 205
The Plural 224
An OT Account 232
Necessary Constraints 232
Inputs in Optimality Theory and Correspondence Theory 238
A Paradox for OT 240
Morphemic Tiers and OT 258
[a]-Initial Verbs 285
The Exceptions 301
Summary 310
4. Alternative Accounts in OT 313
A Linear Planes Analysis 315
Avoiding the Paradox with Levels 320
Problems with a Level-ordered Approach 358
Ad Hoc Levels 361
Consonant Co-occurrence Restrictions 362
Assimilation Constraints 369
5. Some Final Thoughts 403
Huave 405
Arabic 414
Optimal Domains and the Multiplanar Model 417
Possibilities for Axininca Campa 419
Conclusions 423
References 425
Appendix A 430
Abbreviations used 430
ABSTRACT
In this dissertation I show that the representations in Optimality Theory must be extended to allow multiple planes. The variable-position affixes in Afar occur as either prefixes or suffixes depending on the initial segment of the verb root. If the root begins with [i], [e(e)], [o(o)] or [u], the affix is a prefix (e.g., t-okm-e# (2-eat-perf) ‘You (sg.) ate’); if it begins with [a] or a consonant, the affix is a suffix (e.g., rab-t-e# (die-2-perf) ‘You died’). Additionally, plural not only appears as a prefix or a suffix, but when a suffix it can either precede or follow aspect (rab-n-e# (die-pl-perf) ‘I died’ vs. rab-e#-n (die-perf-pl) ‘They died’). A parallel model in Optimality Theory is unable to account for the different order of affixes in forms such as rab-n-e# vs.rab-e#-n. The Multiplanar Model, which posits that output representations consist not only of a word plane but also an affix plane is able to account for this data. The representations for the two forms are: [y-e-n], [rab-e-n] and [n-e], [rab-n-e]. In the first case, plural is specified as the rightmost morpheme by morphological constraints. Even though /y/, the third person marker, cannot surface on the word plane, it satisfies onset on the affix plane. This contrasts with the second case, where onset, being higher ranked than plural (r), requires that plural occurs to the left of aspect to fill the onset position on the affix plane. I then show a serial monoplanar model can also account for this data. Finally, I compare the serial and multiplanar models, arguing that phonological evidence supports the Multiplanar Model.